0:16
Well, basically most of the time, players don't get the hot hand and
the teams don't have significant momentum.
How can we approach this problem mathematically?
Well, let's look at a sequence of wins and losses.
Let's look at the 2013 Red Sox, who closed quickly,
closed strong at the end of the season and basically made the playoffs and
won the world championship which was great because of the Boston marathon tragic.
Boston probably needed that more than any other city, whether or
not your a Red Sox fan or not.
But let's a sequence of wins and losses.
Or it could be shots made or shots missed.
But how could you tell if you have excessive?
Well let's suppose a team went 81-81.
If you saw 81 wins followed by 81 losses
that would indicate streakiness, right?
That's extreme streakiness.
Now momentum, they got red hot, and then they got really cold.
Okay, but if you had win, loss, win, loss for the whole season,
that would be like no streakiness.
2:07
Runs test, to see what a normal number of runs given how many wins and
loses are in your data set, okay?
And if there is no streakiness, the mean number of runs should be two times
the number of wins times the number of losses divided by N is the number of
games, plus one.
Okay, and then the standard deviation would be the mean minus
one times the mean minus two divided by games minus one.
The square root of the that whole thing.
2:40
Okay.
And now how do we tell if something is unusually high or
low on the number of runs?
Again, low number of indicates streakiness.
A high number of runs sort of anti streakiness.
Well, we use what we know about the normal random variable.
The number of runs usually comes close to following a normal random variable.
So anything that's more than two standard deviations above expected,
or two standard deviations below expected,
would indicate something significant without getting really too technical.
That's called the Z-score.
When you take a random variable, the value minus the mean,
and you divide by the standard deviation, that's called a Z-score.
And essentially a lot of statistics is about when you compute the Z-score,
it is greater than plus 2 or minus 2, and that sort of indicates significance.
Again, that comes from the normal random variable, the fact that 95% of the time,
we're basically within two standard deviations.
So let's apply this idea to the 2013 Red Sox.
So for baseball reference, we've got every game did they win or lose.
3:45
And near the end of the season, well they had a 5-game winning streak.
They lost one, won two.
Lost one, won three, lost two.
So, I mean, they got fairly hot at the end of the season.
Okay, so let's see how many runs there were in the other sequence,
consecutive sequences of wins and losses.
What was the expected number and the standard deviation?
Get a Z-score and then we'll see there wasn't a significant result based on
the plus two standard deviations or minus two standard deviations threshold.
And then we'll talk a little bit about the hot hand in basketball, a classic paper by
again, the great, great late psychologist Amos Tversky and his colleagues.
4:50
How many wins, oops,
my keyboard is failing.
97.
Now the loses must be 65 because there's a 162 games.
But let's try, quote L.
Okay, now what's the mean number?
Okay, the mean number is going to be 2 times the wins times the losses and
I gave these range names divided by, in this case 162 plus 1.
So that'd be 79, we'd expect to see 79 runs, or
basically a run again is a consecutive sequence of Ws or Ls.
Now the standard deviation is the square root of means minus one.
6:13
Okay.
Now how many actual runs are there?
Well, here we have to use instinct.
So we'd start out, there's one run, whether it's W or L.
Now how do we know how many runs we have?
Well, basically, if this matches the outcome before, then
we have the same number of runs as before otherwise we have one more run stored.
7:02
So we saw 86 runs and the expected was 79.
We divide by that standard deviation.
And we get that 1.17.
So what we saw, was actually anti-streakiness
because there weren't less runs, there were more runs than were expected.
But it wasn't that unusual.
It was about 1.2 standard deviations above average.
So there wasn't significant streakiness here and
you rarely will see significant streakiness.
7:39
And that's an explanation perhaps for
a lot of people who think they live in a cancer cluster.
because there's going to be some part of the country with a higher
than average cancer rate, just as there could be a part of the country
with a lower than average cancer rate.
So let's talk a little bit about the hot hand in basketball and
there's been some recent research saying maybe there really is the hot hand.
You ask anybody who plays basketball,
do you think your more likely to make the next shot if you made the last shot and
when I ask my students this 90% of the hands go up, of course I'm getting hot.
Well, it just turns out I think not to be true.
So in this classic hot hand in basketball paper from the cognitive
psychology journal in 1985, okay, here's an example of some of the data,
they use more sophisticated tests than runs tests.
Okay, but basically, look at some famous NBA players free throw shoot, so
if you've got the hot of hand, if you made the first foul shot you should be more
likely to make the second foul shot and if he missed the first foul shot.
So for example, Larry Bird, when he missed the first foul shot,
he made the second one 91%.
When he made the first foul shot, he made the second one 88%.
And you just don't see much difference here.
And the difference is really aren't significant for any of these players,
given the number of foul shots that they took, and you basically see this a lot.
Matter of fact, there might be an anti hot hand that you're more likely
to miss the next shot if you made the last shot.
Why might that be?
Because you might think you're in the zone so you're red hot, so
you take a worse shot than you should've and then you're more likely to miss it.
Okay, so be wary when you hear the announcer say, this player's red hot,
although Steph Curry looks like he's always red hot, to be honest.
Okay, that's because he just makes every shot it seems to me.
Okay, but basically when your local baseball or
basketball announcer says, wow, our team is red hot, we've got the momentum.
If you go ahead and do the runs test, I bet you'll find out that basically this
streakiness is well within the bounds of randomness.